دوره 17، شماره 2 - ( 10-1401 )                   جلد 17 شماره 2 صفحات 73-58 | برگشت به فهرست نسخه ها

XML English Abstract Print


1- گروه روان‌شناسی عمومی، سازمان آموزش و پرورش استان زنجان، زنجان، ایران
2- گروه روان‌شناسی دانشگاه زنجان، زنجان، ایران ، javadsalehi@znu.ac.ir
چکیده:   (974 مشاهده)
محدودیت­های حرکتی کودکان دارای ناتوانیهای جسمی جابجایی، اکتشاف محیط و تعاملات اجتماعی آنها را به طور منفی تحت تاثیر قرار داده زمینه ساز تاخیر در رشد شناخت اجتماعی، به ویژه نظریه ذهن(TOM)، در آنها می­شود. پژوهش حاضر با هدف مقایسه سطح توانش­های نظریه ذهن در دانش­آموزان دارای مشکل حرکتی، دانش­آموزان کم­توان ذهنی و دانش­آموزان با رشد طبیعی، با توجه خاص به نقش ناتوانی­های حرکتی انجام شد. در این پژوهش علّی-مقایسه­ای برای دستیابی به این هدف تعداد 44 دانش­آموز فلج مغزی، 29 دانش­آموز کم­توان ذهنی، و 46 دانش­آموز دارای رشد طبیعی به روش دردسترس از مدارس زنجان نمونه ­گیری شدند. از سیستم طبقه­بندی عملکرد حرکتی درشت توسعه یافته و بازبینی شده (GMFCS - E&R)،  فرم 38 سوالی آزمون نظریه‏ی ذهن، و نتایج آزمونهای هوش وکسلر، لایتر و گودیناف (موجود در پرونده کودکان) برای دستیابی به داده­های مورد نیاز استفاده شد. نتایج تحلیل واریانس چند متغیره نشان داد که دانش­آموزان معلول حرکتی به رغم همگن­سازی از لحاظ جنسیت، سن و هوش در نظریه ذهن (به ویژه در سطح پیشرفته) عملکرد ضعیف­تری نسبت به دانش­آموزان عادی از خود نشان دادند. البته عملکرد دانش­آموزان دارای ناتوانی حرکتی از دانش­آموزان کم­توان ذهنی بهتر بود. در مجموع، نتایج ارتباط بین محدودیت حرکتی و نظریه ذهن را تایید نمود و نشان داد که محدودیت حرکتی می­تواند از طریق محدود ساختن روابط اجتماعی فرد، علیرغم داشتن هوش طبیعی، توانش­های نظریه ذهن او را تحت تاثیر منفی قرار دهد.
متن کامل [PDF 1119 kb]   (19 دریافت)    
نوع مطالعه: پژوهشي | موضوع مقاله: تخصصي
دریافت: 1399/9/4 | پذیرش: 1402/8/15 | انتشار: 1403/4/6

فهرست منابع
1. Abbeduto, L., Short-Meyerson, K., Benson, G., & Dolish, J. (2004). Relationship between theory of mind and language ability in children and adolescents with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 48(2), 150-159. [DOI:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2004.00524.x] [PMID]
2. Alevriadou, A., & Giaouri, S. (2011). Second-order mental state attribution in children with intellectual disability: Cognitive functioning and some educational planning challenges. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology, 1(1), 146-153. [DOI:10.5539/jedp.v1n1p146]
3. American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (AAIDD). (2019). Intellectual disability. Retrieved from https://aaidd.org/intellectual-disability/definition.
4. Astington, J. (1994). Children's Developing Notions of Others' Minds. In J. Duchan, L. Hewitt, & R. Sonnenmeier (Eds.), Pragmatics: From theory to practice (pp. 72-87). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
5. Baron, R. A., & Branscombe, N. R. (2023). Social psychology (15th global edition). Pearson Education.
6. Baurain, C., & Nader-Grosbois, N. (2013). Theory of mind, socio-emotional problem-solving, socio-emotional regulation in children with intellectual disability and in typically developing children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(5), 1080-1097. [DOI:10.1007/s10803-012-1651-4] [PMID]
7. Bax, M., Goldstein, M., Rosenbaum, P., Leviton, A., & Paneth, N. (2005). Proposed definition and classification of cerebral palsy: Introduction. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 47, 571-576. [DOI:10.1017/S001216220500112X] [PMID]
8. Bigelow, F. J., Clark, G. M., Lum, J. A., & Enticott, P. G. (2021). The mediating effect of language on the development of cognitive and affective theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 209, 105-158. [DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105158] [PMID]
9. Brüne, M., Abdel-Hamid, M., Lehmkamper, C., & Sonntag, C. (2007). Mental state attribution, neurocognitive functioning, and psychopathology:
10. What predicts poor social competence in schizophrenia best? Schizophrenia Research, 92, 151-159.
11. Buckley, S., Bird, G., & Sacks, B. (2002). Social development for individuals with Down syndrome-An overview. Down Syndrome Issues and Information. Advance online publication.
12. Caillies, S., Hody, A., & Calmus, A. (2012). Theory of mind and irony comprehension in children with cerebral palsy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 1380-1388. [DOI:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.03.012] [PMID]
13. Canino, S., Raimo, S., Boccia, M., Di Vita, A., & Palermo, L. (2022). On the embodiment of social cognition skills: The inner and outer body processing differently contributes to the affective and cognitive theory of mind. Brain Sciences, 12(11), 1423. [DOI:10.3390/brainsci12111423] [PMID] []
14. Cobos, J. F. M., & Castro, M. C. A. (2010). Theory of mind in young people with Down syndrome. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 10(3), 363-385.
15. Cole, M. P., Dennis, T. A., Smith-Simon, K. E., & Cohen, L. H. (2008). Preschoolers' emotion regulation strategy understanding: Relations with emotion socialization and child self-regulation. Social Development, 18(2), 324-352. [DOI:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00503.x]
16. Dahlgren Sandberg, A., & Dahlgren, S. (2012). Theory of mind in children with cerebral palsy: The impact of limited expressive linguistic abilities. In Access to Language and Cognitive Development (pp. 62-80). [DOI:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199592722.003.0004] [PMID]
17. Dahlgren, S. O., Dahlgren-Sandberg, A., & Larsson, M. (2010). Theory of mind in children with severe speech and physical impairments. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 617-624. [DOI:10.1016/j.ridd.2009.12.010] [PMID]
18. Deelstra, J. (2021). The role of working memory in theory of mind: A behavioral study coupled with EEG hyperscanning (Doctoral dissertation).
19. Dennis, M., Simic, N., Bigler, E. D., Abildskov, T., Agostino, A., Taylor, H. G., et al. (2013). Cognitive, affective, and conative theory of mind (ToM) in children with traumatic brain injury. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 25-39. [DOI:10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.006] [PMID] []
20. De Villiers, J. G., & De Villiers, P. A. (2014). The role of language in theory of mind development. Topics in Language Disorders, 34(4), 313-328. [DOI:10.1097/TLD.0000000000000037]
21. Dix, L. E. (2016). Exploring the development of theory of mind in children with Down syndrome (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). The University of Leeds School of Education, Leeds, England.
22. Dodge, K. A., Laird, R., Lochman, J. E., & Zelli, A. (2002). Multidimensional latent-construct analysis of children's social information processing patterns: Correlations with aggressive behavior problems. Psychological Assessment, 14(1), 60-73. [DOI:10.1037/1040-3590.14.1.60] [PMID] []
23. Falkman, K. W., Dahlgren Sandberg, A., & Hjelmquist, E. (2005). Theory of mind in children with severe speech and physical impairment (SSPI): A longitudinal study. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 52, 139-157. [DOI:10.1080/10349120500086397]
24. Farhangi Nasab, F. F., Purpirali, M., Shams, M., & Yaghini, O. (2020). Comparison of the levels of theory of mind between students with cerebral palsy and healthy students in Isfahan. Journal of Clinical Physiotherapy Research, 5(2), 14. (In Persian)
25. Fiasse, C., & Nader-Grosbois, N. (2012). Perceived social acceptance, theory of mind and social adjustment in children with intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(6), 1871-1880. [DOI:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.05.017] [PMID]
26. Frith, U. (1996). Social communication and its disorder in autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 10, 48-53. [DOI:10.1177/026988119601000108] [PMID]
27. Ghamarani, A., & Alborzi, S. (2005). Developmental study of theory of mind in mildly mentally retarded children and normal children aged 7 to 9. Psychological Studies, 1(4 & 5), 5-28. (In Persian)
28. Ghamarani, A., Alborzi, S., & Khayer, M. (2006). Examining the validity and reliability of the theory of mind test in a group of mentally retarded and normal students in Shiraz. Psychology, 38(2), 181-199. (In Persian)
29. Guralnick, M., Connor, R., Neville, B., & Hammond, M. (2006). Promoting the peer-related social development of young children with mild developmental delays: Effectiveness of a comprehensive intervention. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 111(5), 336-356. [DOI:10.1352/0895-8017(2006)111[336:PTPSDO]2.0.CO;2] [PMID]
30. Haresabadi, F., Jafarzadeh, H., Rostami, M., Abbasi Shayeh, Z., Maleki Shahmahmood, T., Enayati, S., Mirnejhad, M., Ghavami, M., & Mashhadi, A. (2021). Comparing theory of mind skills and language performance between children with developmental language disorder, high-functioning autism, and typically developing children. Journal of Mazandaran University of Medical Sciences, 31(195), 37-55. (In Persian)
31. Houwen, S., Putten, A., & Vlaskamp, C. (2014). A systematic review of the effects of motor interventions to improve motor, cognitive, and/or social functioning in people with severe or profound intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 2093-2116. [DOI:10.1016/j.ridd.2014.05.006] [PMID]
32. Jacobs, E., & Nader-Grosbois, N. (2020). Affective and cognitive theory of mind in children with intellectual disabilities: How to train them to foster social adjustment and emotion regulation. Journal of Education and Training Studies, 8, 80-97. [DOI:10.11114/jets.v8i4.4757]
33. Ketelaars, M. P., Van Weerdenburg, M., Verhoeven, L., Cuperus, J. M., & Jansonius, K. (2010). Dynamics of the Theory of Mind construct: Developmental perspective. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(1), 85-103. [DOI:10.1080/17405620903482081]
34. Kosinski, M. (2023). Theory of Mind May Have Spontaneously Emerged in Large Language Models. arXiv preprint arXiv:2302.02083.
35. Koskinen, S. (1998). Quality of life 10 years after a very severe traumatic brain injury (TBI): The perspective of the injured and the closest relative. Brain Injury, 12, 631-648. [DOI:10.1080/026990598122205] [PMID]
36. Koski, S. E., & Steck, E. H. M. (2010). Empathic chimpanzees: Proposal of the levels of emotional and cognitive processing in chimpanzee empathy. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(1), 38-66. [DOI:10.1080/17405620902986991]
37. Leon-Carrion, J., Taaffe, P. J., & Barroso y Martin, J. M. (2006). Neuropsychological assessment of persons with acquired brain injury. In J. Leon-Carrion, K. R. H. von Wild, & G. A. Zitney (Eds.), Brain injury treatment: Theories and Practices (pp. 275-312). London & New York: Taylor & Francis. [DOI:10.4324/9780203965238]
38. Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
39. Li, X., Wang, K., Wu, J., Hong, Y., Zhao, J., Feng, X., Xu, M., Wang, M., Ndasauka, Y., & Zhang, X. (2014). The link between impaired theory of mind and executive function in children with cerebral palsy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 1686-1693. [DOI:10.1016/j.ridd.2014.03.017] [PMID]
40. Lillard, A. S., Pinkham, A. M., & Smith, E. (2011). Pretend play and cognitive development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (2nd ed., pp. 335-353). Wiley-Blackwell. [DOI:10.1002/9781444325485.ch11]
41. Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2005). Evaluating the causes of impaired irony comprehension following traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology, 19, 712-730. [DOI:10.1080/02687030500172203]
42. McDonald, S. (2013). Impairments in social cognition following severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(3), 231-246. [DOI:10.1017/S1355617712001506] [PMID]
43. Merrell, K. W., & Holland, M. L. (1997). Social-emotional behavior of preschool-age children with and without developmental delays. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18(6), 393-405. [DOI:10.1016/S0891-4222(97)00018-8] [PMID]
44. Milders, M. (2019). Relationship between social cognition and social behaviour following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 33(1), 62-68. [DOI:10.1080/02699052.2018.1531301] [PMID]
45. Muris, P., Steerneman, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., Horselenberg, R., van den Hogen, T., & Dongen, L. (1999). The TOM test: A new instrument for assessing Theory of Mind in normal children and children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 67-80. [DOI:10.1023/A:1025922717020] [PMID]
46. Neitzel, I., & Penke, M. (2021). Theory of Mind in children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 113, 103945. [DOI:10.1016/j.ridd.2021.103945] [PMID]
47. Nesayan, A., Asadi Gandomani, R., & Pahlavan Mazar, H. (2016). Comparison of Theory of Mind in Children with Intellectual Disability and Preschool and Its Relation with Social Maturity. Journal of Disability Studies, 6(12), 105-111.
48. Ornaghi, V., Pepe, A., & Grazzani, I. (2016). False-belief understanding and language ability mediate the relationship between emotion comprehension and prosocial orientation in preschoolers. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1534. [DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01534] [PMID] []
49. Palisano, R. J., Rosenbaum, P., Bartlett, D., & Livingston, M. H. (2008). Content validity of the expanded and revised Gross Motor Function Classification System. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 50, 744-750. [DOI:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03089.x] [PMID]
50. Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 515-526. [DOI:10.1017/S0140525X00076512]
51. Riahi, A., Rassafiani, M., & Binesh, M. (2013). The Cross-Cultural Validation and Test-Retest and Inter-Rater Reliability of the Persian Translation of Parent Version of the Gross Motor Function Classification System for Children with Cerebral Palsy. Rehabilitation, 13(5), 25-30. (In Persian)
52. Rosello, B., Berenguer, C., Baixauli, I., García, R., & Miranda, A. (2020). Theory of mind profiles in children with autism spectrum disorder: Adaptive/social skills and pragmatic competence. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 567401. [DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.567401] [PMID] []
53. Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children's and mothers' mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73(3), 734-751. [DOI:10.1111/1467-8624.00435] [PMID]
54. Sadowska, M., Sarecka-Hujar, B., & Kopyta, I. (2020). Cerebral palsy: Current opinions on definition, epidemiology, risk factors, classification and treatment options. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 16, 1505-1518. [DOI:10.2147/NDT.S235165] [PMID] []
55. Santoro, J., & Spiers, M. (1994). Social cognitive factors in brain injury-associated personality change. Brain Injury, 8, 265-276. [DOI:10.3109/02699059409150978] [PMID]
56. Smogorzewska, J., & Osterhaus, C. (2021). Advanced theory of mind in children with mild intellectual disability and deaf or hard of hearing children: A two-year longitudinal study in middle childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 603-624. [DOI:10.1111/bjdp.12389] [PMID]
57. Smogorzewska, J., Szumski, G., & Grygiel, P. (2018). Same or different? Theory of mind among children with and without disabilities. PLOS ONE, 13(10), e0205583. [DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0202553] [PMID] []
58. Spatt, J., Zebenholzer, K., & Oder, W. (1997). Psychosocial long-term outcome of severe head injury as perceived by patients, relatives, and professionals. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 95, 173-179. [DOI:10.1111/j.1600-0404.1997.tb00091.x] [PMID]
59. Tabachnick, B. G., Fidell, L. S., & Ullman, J. B. (2019). Using Multivariate Statistics (7th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.
60. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1994). Predicting and explaining behavior: A comparison of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1059-1075. [DOI:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01809.x] [PMID]
61. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2005). How Language Facilitates the Acquisition of False-Belief Understanding in Children with Autism. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind (pp. 298-318). Oxford University Press. [DOI:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0014]
62. Tajadini, S., Farpour, S., Farpour, H., & Nikpour, F. (2021). Interrelationships between Theory of Mind and Language Skills in Children with Cerebral Palsy: A Review of the Literature. Journal of Rehabilitation Sciences & Research, 8(3), 101-105. (In Persian)
63. Thirion-Marissiaux, A. F., & Nader-Grosbois, N. (2008). Theory of mind and socio-affective abilities in disabled children and adolescents. European Journal of Disability Research, Alter, 2(2), 133-155. [DOI:10.1016/j.alter.2008.02.003]
64. Voorman, J. M., Dallmeijer, A. J., Van Eck, M., Schuengel, C., & Becher, J. G. (2010). Social functioning and communication in children with cerebral palsy: Association with disease characteristics and personal and environmental factors. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 52, 441-447. [DOI:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03399.x] [PMID]
65. Yates, P. J. (2003). Psychological adjustment, social enablement and community integration following acquired brain injury. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 13, 367-387. [DOI:10.1080/09602010244000408] [PMID]
66. Zelazo, P. D., Burack, J. A., Benedetto, E., & Frye, D. (1996). Theory of mind and rule use in individuals with Down syndrome: A test of the uniqueness and specificity claims. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 479-484. [DOI:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01429.x] [PMID]
67. Zion, E., & Jenvey, V. B. (2006). Temperament and social behaviour at home and school among typically developing children and children with an intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50(6), 445-456. [DOI:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00790.x] [PMID]

ارسال پیام به نویسنده مسئول


بازنشر اطلاعات
Creative Commons License این مقاله تحت شرایط Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License قابل بازنشر است.

کلیه حقوق این وب سایت متعلق به روانشناسی معاصر،دوفصلنامه انجمن روانشناسی ایران می باشد.

طراحی و برنامه نویسی : یکتاوب افزار شرق